Skip to main content
Intended for healthcare professionals
Restricted access
Research article
First published online July 11, 2022

Speak Up! Mistaken Beliefs About How Much to Talk in Conversations

Abstract

We hypothesized that people would exhibit a reticence bias, the incorrect belief that they will be more likable if they speak less than half the time in a conversation with a stranger, as well as halo ignorance, the belief that their speaking time should depend on their goal (e.g., to be liked vs. to be found interesting), when in fact, perceivers form global impressions of each other. In Studies 1 and 2, participants forecasted they should speak less than half the time when trying to be liked, but significantly more when trying to be interesting. In Study 3, we tested the accuracy of these forecasts by randomly assigning participants to speak for 30%, 40%, 50%, 60%, or 70% of the time in a dyadic conversation. Contrary to people’s forecasts, they were more likable the more they spoke, and their partners formed global rather than differentiated impressions.

Get full access to this article

View all access and purchase options for this article.

Data availability statement

The raw data from the studies can be found at: https://osf.io/uajw3/

References

Ames D., Maissen L. B., Brockner J. (2012). The role of listening in interpersonal influence. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(3), 345–349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.010
Aron A., Melinat E., Aron E. N., Vallone R. D., Bator R. J. (1997). The experimental generation of interpersonal closeness: A procedure and some preliminary findings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(4), 363–377. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167297234003
Bates D., Maechler M., Bolker B., Walker S. (2021). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4 (Version 1.1-27.1) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
Ben-Shachar M., Makowski D., Lüdecke D. (2021). effectsize: Indices of effect size and standardized parameters (Version 0.4.5) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=effectsize
Blumberg H. H. (1972). Communication of interpersonal evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23(2), 157–162. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033027
Boothby E. J., Cooney G., Sandstrom G. M., Clark M. S. (2018). The liking gap in conversations: Do people like us more than we think? Psychological Science, 29(11), 1742–1756. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618783714
Carlson E., Kenny D. A. (2012). Meta-accuracy: Do we know how others see us? In Vazire S., Wilson T. D. (Eds.), Handbook of self-knowledge (pp. 242–257). Guilford Press.
Carnegie D. (1936). How to win friends and influence people. Simon & Schuster.
Chambers J. R., Epley N., Savitsky K., Windschitl P. D. (2008). Knowing too much: Using private knowledge to predict how one is viewed by others. Psychological Science, 19(6), 542–548. https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1467-9280.2008.02121.x
Cooney G., Gilbert D. T., Wilson T. D. (2017). The novelty penalty: Why do people like talking about new experiences but hearing about old ones? Psychological Science, 28(3), 380–394. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616685870
Diener E., Seligman M. E. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13(1), 81–84. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00415
Epley N., Eyal T. (2019). Through a looking glass, darkly: Using mechanisms of mind perception to identify accuracy, overconfidence, and underappreciated means for improvement. In Olsen J. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 60, pp. 65–120). Elsevier.
Epley N., Schroeder J. (2014). Mistakenly seeking solitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(5), 1980–1999. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037323
Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A.-G., Buchner A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
Finkel E. J., Norton M. I., Reis H. T., Ariely D., Caprariello P. A., Eastwick P. W., Frost J. H., Maniaci M. R. (2015). When does familiarity promote versus undermine interpersonal attraction? A proposed integrative model from erstwhile adversaries. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 3–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614561682
Garner D. (2011, October 5). Classic advice: Please, leave well enough alone. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2011/10/05/books/books-of-the-times-classic-advice-please-leave-well-enough-alone.html
Gordon R. A. (1996). Impact of ingratiation on judgments and evaluations: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(1), 54–70. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.1.54
Harvard Medical School. (2007). National Comorbidity Survey (NCS). https://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/ncs/index.php
Hirschi Q., Wilson T. D., Gilbert D. T. (2020). Forecasts about a naturalistic conversation [Unpublished raw data]. Department of Psychology, University of Virginia.
Huang K., Yeomans M., Brooks A. W., Minson J., Gino F. (2017). It doesn’t hurt to ask: Question-asking increases liking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(3), 430–452. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000097
Ickes W. (2003). Everyday mind reading: Understanding what other people think and feel. Prometheus Books.
Jones E. E. (1964). Ingratiation. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Jones E. E., Wortman C. B. (1973). Ingratiation: An attributional approach. General Learning Press.
Kardas M., Kumar A., Epley N. (2022). Overly shallow? Miscalibrated expectations create a barrier to deeper conversation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122(3), 367–398. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000281
Kassambara A. (2021). rstatix: Pipe-friendly framework for basic statistical tests (Version 0.7.0) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rstatix
Kenny D. A. (2019). Interpersonal perception: The foundation of social relationships. Guilford Press.
Lance C. E., LaPointe J. A., Fisicaro S. A. (1994). Tests of three causal models of halo rater error. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 57(1), 83–96. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1994.1005
Leary M. R., Rogers P. A., Canfield R. W., Coe C. (1986). Boredom in interpersonal encounters: Antecedents and social implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(5), 968–975. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.5.968
Lenth R. (2021). emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means (Version 1.6.2-1) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans
Lloyd K. J., Boer D., Kluger A. N., Voelpel S. C. (2015). Building trust and feeling well: Examining intraindividual and interpersonal outcomes and underlying mechanisms of listening. International Journal of Listening, 29(1), 12–29. https://doi.org/10.1080/10904018.2014.928211
Lopez-Rosenfeld M., Calero C. I., Fernandez Slezak D., Garbulsky G., Bergman M., Trevisan M., Sigman M. (2015). Neglect in human communication: Quantifying the cost of cell-phone interruptions in face to face dialogs. PLOS ONE, 10(6), Article e0125772. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0125772
Nisbett R. E., Wilson T. D. (1977). The halo effect: Evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(4), 250–256. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.35.4.250
Sandstrom G. M., Boothby E., Cooney G. (2021). Talking to strangers—A week-long intervention reduces psychological barriers to social connection. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/29q8j
Sandstrom G. M., Tseng V. W.-S., Costa J., Okeke F., Choudhury T., Dunn E. W. (2016). Talking less during social interactions predicts enjoyment: A mobile sensing pilot study. PLOS ONE, 11(7), Article e0158834. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158834
Savitsky K., Epley N., Gilovich T. (2001). Do others judge us as harshly as we think? Overestimating the impact of our failures, shortcomings, and mishaps. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 44–56. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.44
Schug J., Yuki M., Maddux W. (2010). Relational mobility explains between- and within-culture differences in self-disclosure to close friends. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1471–1478. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610382786
Siedlecki K. L., Salthouse T. A., Oishi S., Jeswani S. (2014). The relationship between social support and subjective well-being across age. Social Indicators Research, 117(2), 561–576. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-013-0361-4
Sprecher S., Treger S., Wondra J. D. (2012). Effects of self-disclosure role on liking, closeness, and other impressions in get-acquainted interactions. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 30(4), 497–514. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407512459033
Stopa L., Clark D. M. (2000). Social phobia and interpretation of social events. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(3), 273–283. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00043-1
Swann W. B., Stein-Seroussi A., McNulty S. E. (1992). Outcasts in a white-lie society: The enigmatic worlds of people with negative self-conceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(4), 618–624. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.4.618
Van Lange P. A., Columbus S. (2021). Vitamin S: Why is social contact, even with strangers, so important to well-being? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30(3), 267–273. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211002538
Van Quaquebeke N., Felps W. (2018). Respectful inquiry: A motivational account of leading through asking questions and listening. Academy of Management Review, 43(1), 5–27. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2014.0537
Weger H., Castle G. R., Emmett M. C. (2010). Active listening in peer interviews: The influence of message paraphrasing on perceptions of listening skill. International Journal of Listening, 24(1), 34–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/10904010903466311
Welker C., Walker J. T., Boothby E. J., Gilovich T. (2022). Pessimistic assessments of ability in everyday conversation. Cornell University [Unpublished manuscript].

Supplementary Material

Please find the following supplemental material available below.

For Open Access articles published under a Creative Commons License, all supplemental material carries the same license as the article it is associated with.

For non-Open Access articles published, all supplemental material carries a non-exclusive license, and permission requests for re-use of supplemental material or any part of supplemental material shall be sent directly to the copyright owner as specified in the copyright notice associated with the article.

Cite article

Cite article

Cite article

OR

Download to reference manager

If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice

Share options

Share

Share this article

Share with email
EMAIL ARTICLE LINK
Share on social media

Share access to this article

Sharing links are not relevant where the article is open access and not available if you do not have a subscription.

For more information view the Sage Journals article sharing page.

Information, rights and permissions

Information

Published In

Article first published online: July 11, 2022
Issue published: October 2023

Keywords

  1. conversation
  2. meta-perception
  3. social perception
  4. interpersonal perception
  5. affective forecasting

Rights and permissions

© 2022 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
Request permissions for this article.

Data availability statement

Data is available for this article. View more information
PubMed: 35818304

Authors

Affiliations

Timothy D. Wilson
University of Virginia, Charlottesville, USA
Daniel T. Gilbert
Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA

Notes

Quinn Hirschi, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box 400400, Charlottesville, VA 22904-4400, USA. Email: [email protected]

Metrics and citations

Metrics

Journals metrics

This article was published in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.

VIEW ALL JOURNAL METRICS

Article usage*

Total views and downloads: 1857

*Article usage tracking started in December 2016


Altmetric

See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores



Articles citing this one

Receive email alerts when this article is cited

Web of Science: 4 view articles Opens in new tab

Crossref: 0

  1. Listening and the pursuit of communal relationships
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  2. Listening and responsiveness in getting-acquainted processes
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  3. Talking to strangers: Intention, competence, and opportunity
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar

Figures and tables

Figures & Media

Tables

View Options

Get access

Access options

If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:

SPSP members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.

SPSP members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.


Alternatively, view purchase options below:

Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.

Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Full Text

View Full Text